The largest database of trusted experimental protocols
> Physiology > Mental Process > Memory Consolidation

Memory Consolidation

Memory consolidation is the process by which temporary memories are transformed into long-term, stable memories.
This process involves the strengthening of neural connections and the reorganization of information in the brain.
It is a crucial step in the formation of lasting memories, and is influenced by various factors such as sleep, stress, and neurochemical activity.
Understanding the mechanisms of memory consolidation is an active area of research, with implications for cognitive function, learning, and the development of treatments for memory disorders.

Most cited protocols related to «Memory Consolidation»

Protocol full text hidden due to copyright restrictions

Open the protocol to access the free full text link

Publication 2011
EPOCH protocol Memory Consolidation Patients Sleep Sleep, REM Sleep Stages Strains
Our primary collection of literature was gathered by searching online electronic databases for articles relevant to our topic of interest through December 2008. The four databases used were PsycINFO, Medline, Web of Science, and EBSCO MegaFile. In each of these databases, we conducted a search using a combination of the following terms: sleep deprivation or sleep restriction and cognition, attention, memory, performance, vigilance, and executive function (12 combinations in all). This search yielded 4,276 hits in total. We next scanned the abstracts of these articles to determine their suitability for inclusion in the analysis. In total, 176 of the articles were empirical studies that employed SD as a primary independent variable and used at least one cognitive measure as a dependent outcome. We subsequently obtained the full text of these articles to determine whether they met full inclusion criteria. These criteria were as follows:
Because of the restrictions imposed by Criterion 3, a number of subareas within the realm of SD research necessarily had to be omitted from this analysis. A survey of the literature on SD and decision making revealed that outcome variables on these tests did not form a cluster that was homogeneous enough to warrant a quantitative synthesis. This was because many of these experiments employed complex, real-world scenarios, opting for ecologically valid designs over more controlled neuropsychological tests (for a review, see Harrison & Horne, 2000 (link)). Moreover, it is unclear how outcome variables from standardized decision-making tests (e.g., the Iowa Gambling Test) should be compared with the accuracy measures obtained from other cognitive domains. Finally, experiments on implicit and procedural memory were also excluded, as these form a separate body of literature pertaining to sleep and memory consolidation (Stickgold & Walker, 2005 (link)), the analysis of which is beyond the scope of this article.
In addition to this online literature search, we obtained data from several other sources. We conducted hand searches of the journal Sleep and the Journal of Sleep Research from 1988 to 2008. We also reviewed the reference lists of the major review articles on SD and cognitive performance that have been published over the last several years. Finally, to mitigate the “file drawer” problem (Strube & Hartmann, 1983 ), we contacted eight major sleep laboratories conducting research in this field to request unpublished data from experiments, as well as master’s and doctoral theses. We received additional data from one of these laboratories, as well as replies from all but one of the remaining investigators informing us that they did not have suitable data for inclusion. In total, 70 articles and 147 data sets met inclusion criteria and were included in the meta-analysis (see Table 1). Among these, numerous data sets contained more than one cognitive outcome; these were coded separately, according to the recommendations of Hunter and Schmidt (2004) . Altogether, 209 aggregated effect sizes and 5,104 individual effect sizes were calculated from these data sets.
Publication 2010
Anabolism Attention Cognition Executive Function Human Body Memory Memory Consolidation Neuropsychological Tests Physicians Sleep Wakefulness Walkers
Cyclodextrin-encapsulated 17β-estradiol (E2; Sigma, St. Louis, MO) at a dose of 0.2 mg/kg was dissolved in physiological saline in a volume of 4 ml/kg, and injected intraperitoneally (i.p.). This dose in mice facilitates object memory consolidation in the task used here (Gresack and Frick, 2004 (link), 2006 (link)). The vehicle, hydroxypropyl-β-cyclodextrin (HBC), was dissolved in an equal volume of saline and contained the same amount of cyclodextrin as E2. The MEK inhibitor SL327 (α-[Amino[(4-aminophenyl)thio]methylene]-2-(trifluoromethyl) benzeneacetonitrile; Sigma), at a dose of 30 mg/kg, was dissolved in 100% dimethyl sulfoxide (DMSO) and injected i.p. in a volume of 2.0 ml/kg. Vehicle controls received HBC or both HBC and DMSO. For intrahippocampal (IH) infusions, physiological saline or cyclodextrin-encapsulated E2 dissolved in physiological saline (5.0 μg/0.5μl) was infused at 0.5 μl/min for 1 minute.
To demonstrate that E2-induced increases in object recognition were dependent on dorsal hippocampal ERK activation, other mice received IH infusions of vehicle or the MEK inhibitor U0126 (1,4-Diamino-2,3-dicyano-1,4-bis (o-aminophenylmercapto) butadiene; 2.0 μg/μl; Sigma) concurrently with i.p. E2 injection or intracerebroventricular (ICV) infusion of bovine serum albumin-conjugated 17β-estradiol (see below) into the dorsal third ventricle. U0126 was dissolved in 100% DMSO to 4 μg/μl as a stock solution and then serially diluted in physiological saline for infusion of various doses. U0126 or vehicle (50% DMSO) were infused at a rate of 0.50 μl/min and a volume of 0.50 μl/side. Other mice received ICV infusions of vehicle or bovine serum albumin-conjugated 17β-estradiol (β-Estradiol 6-(O-carboxy-methyl)oxime; BSA-E2; Sigma). The covalent conjugation of E2 to the large BSA molecule prevents E2 from passing through the cell membrane and binding to intracellular ERs (Taguchi et al., 2004 (link)). Thus, effects of BSA-E2 should be mediated by membrane-bound ERs. BSA-E2 was dissolved in Tris-HCl to a concentration of 5.0 μM. Either 5.0 μM BSA-E2 or vehicle (Tris-HCl) was infused at a rate of 0.5 μl/min at a volume of 1.0 μl.
To demonstrate that the effects of BSA-E2 on memory and ERK activation were independent of nuclear estrogen receptors, other mice received ICV infusions of BSA-E2 conducted as described above concurrently with IH infusions of the nuclear estrogen receptor antagonist ICI 182,780 ((7a,17b)-7-[9[(4,4,5,5,5pentafluoropentyl) Sulfinyl]nonyl]estra-1,3,5(10)-triene-3,17-diol; 10 μg/μl; Sigma). ICI 182,780 is an antagonist of ERα and ERβ that impairs E2-induced ER dimerization (Weatherman et al., 2002 (link)) and translocation of ERs into the cell nucleus (Dauvois et al., 1993 (link)). If ICI 182,780 does not block the effects of BSA-E2 on memory and ERK activation, then this would indicate that these effects are mediated by membrane-associated estrogen receptors rather than nuclear receptors. Intrahippocampal infusions of ICI 182,780 were conducted at a rate of 0.5 μl/min and a volume of 0.5 μl/side, resulting in a dose of 5.0 μg/side. In other mice, ICI 182,780 was infused intrahippocampally without concurrent ICV infusion as a control due to a report that this compound in female rats can act as an estrogen receptor agonist to enhance place learning when administered in the absence of E2 (Zurkovsky et al., 2006 (link)). To compare effects of ICI 182,780 with those of traditional E2, 0.2 mg/kg E2 was administered i.p. either alone or with ICI 182,780 infused intrahippocampally. The E2 + IH ICI 182,780 group also served as a control for the effectiveness of the ICI compound in blocking the effects of traditional, non-BSA conjugated E2 on object memory consolidation and ERK activation. Vehicle controls for the aforementioned groups received ICV and IH infusions of saline or BSA dissolved in saline.
Finally, to demonstrate that the effects of BSA-E2 on memory and ERK activation involved the dorsal hippocampus, BSA-E2 was infused bilaterally into the dorsal hippocampus of another set of mice. Intrahippocampal infusions were conducted at a rate of 0.5 μl/min and at a volume of 0.5 μl/side, resulting in doses of 5.0 μM/side. As a control, these mice also received ICV infusions of vehicle (BSA dissolved in saline). Additional mice received ICV BSA-E2 + IH vehicle as a control to replicate the effects of ICV BSA-E2 observed above. As an additional method of demonstrating hippocampal involvement in the BSA-E2 effect, other mice received ICV infusions of BSA-E2 conducted as described above concurrently with IH infusions of the GABAA receptor agonist muscimol (3-Hydroxy-5-aminomethyl-isoxazole; 0.50 μg/μl dissolved in saline; Sigma). Muscimol temporarily inactivates a brain region of interest by increasing GABAergic inhibition without permanently damaging the tissue (Martin, 1991 (link)). Therefore, if muscimol interferes with the beneficial effects of ICV BSA-E2 on memory, then this would suggest critical involvement of the dorsal hippocampus in this effect. Intrahippocampal infusions of muscimol were conducted at the same rate and volume as above. Vehicle controls for all of the aforementioned groups received ICV and IH infusions of saline or BSA dissolved in saline.
Injection cannulae for IH and ICV infusions remained in place for 1 minute after infusion to prevent drug diffusion up the injection track. For behaviorally tested mice, all solutions were administered immediately after the sample phase of object recognition training.
Publication 2008
1,3-butadiene Brain Cannula carbene Cell Nucleus Cyclodextrins Diffusion Dimerization Estradiol estradiol-bovine serum albumin Estrogen Nuclear Receptor estrogen receptor alpha, human Estrogen Receptor Antagonists Females GABA-A Receptor Agonists GPER protein, human Hypromellose ICI 182780 Infusions, Intracerebroventricular Isoxazoles Memory Memory Consolidation Mus Muscimol Oximes Pharmaceutical Preparations physiology Plasma Membrane Protoplasm Psychological Inhibition Rattus Receptors, Nuclear Saline Solution Seahorses Serum Albumin, Bovine Sulfoxide, Dimethyl Tissue, Membrane Tissues Translocation, Chromosomal Tromethamine U 0126 Ventricles, Third
To investigate the effect of quercetin on memory functions, we performed a behavioral study (n = 15/group) using a Morris water maze (MWM) task and a Y-maze task.
The MWM test is a parameter task to evaluate memory functions. The experimental apparatus consisted of a circular water tank (100 cm in diameter, 40 cm in height) containing water (23 ± 1°C) to a depth of 15.5 cm, which was rendered opaque by adding white paint. A transparent escape platform (10 cm in diameter, 20 cm in height) was hidden 1 cm below the water surface and placed at the midpoint of one quadrant. The MWM test was started on day 7 and completed on the 13th day of the experimental schedule (Figure 1). Each mouse received training each day for 6 consecutive days using a single hidden platform in one quadrant with three rotating starting quadrants. Latency to escape from the water maze (finding the submerged escape platform) was calculated for each trial. On day seven, final escape latency and probe tests were performed to evaluate memory consolidation. The probe test was performed by removing the platform and allowing each mouse to swim freely for 60 s. The time the mice spent in the target quadrant (where the platform was located during hidden platform training) was measured. The time spent in the target quadrant is considered to represent the degree of memory consolidation that has taken place after learning. All data were recorded using video-tracking software (SMART, Panlab Harvard Apparatus Bioscience Company, United States).
The Y-maze was built from wood that had been painted black. Each arm of the maze was 50 cm long, 20 cm high, and 10 cm wide at the bottom and top. The Y-maze was started on day 12 and completed on day 14 of the experimental schedule (Figure 1). Each mouse was placed at the center of the apparatus and allowed to move freely through the maze for three 8-min sessions. The series of arm entries was visually observed. Spontaneous alteration was defined as the successive entry of the mice into the three arms in overlapping triplet sets. Alteration behavior (%) was calculated as follows: [successive triplet sets (entries into three different arms consecutively)/total number of arm entries-2] × 100.
Full text: Click here
Publication 2018
MAZE protocol Memory Memory Consolidation Mice, House Morris Water Maze Test Quercetin Triplets
In addition to the pre-examination visit, subjects had to report to the laboratory on three other occasions. Figure 1 shows the different phases of the experiment. On the first day subjects performed 10 trials of the AT to measure performance at baseline. After the baseline measurement subjects in the CON group were instructed to rest in bed for 20 min whereas subjects in the PRE group undertook a 20 min intense cycling exercise protocol. After the resting period (CON) or the performance of the exercise bout (PRE) subjects practiced the AT. Subjects in the POST group practiced the AT first and then undertook 20 min of intense cycling. After practice, subjects in the CON and PRE groups were placed in a hospital bed where they rested for 1 hour. To ensure that all groups had the same resting time (1 hour) between the end of practice and the first retention test the POST group rested 40 min after exercise. To minimize changes in arousal and interferences in the consolidation of memory during the resting period, subjects were not allowed to listen to music nor sleep but were allowed to read. Improvements in performance during practice measured the rate of motor skill acquisition. To evaluate the impact of exercise on the short-term retention of the motor skill, a retention test of the AT was performed after the resting period (1 hour). The impact of exercise on the long-term retention was assessed with a retention test 24 hours and 7 days after practicing the AT.
Full text: Click here
Publication 2012
Arousal Auditory Perception Memory Consolidation Motor Skills Rest, Bed Retention (Psychology) Sleep

Most recents protocols related to «Memory Consolidation»

To test spatial mobility, 5 female and 5 male (C57BL/6 mice, aged 12 weeks to 24 weeks) were allowed to freely explore a 60 cm×60 cm open field. In order to minimize external visual cues, the open-field was surrounded by black walls. A single white cue was located on one wall to orient the animal. In order to minimize the effect of external noise, the open field arena was be placed in a 49”(l) × 49”(w) × 102”(h) soundproof studio room (reduces noise above 1000 Hz by greater than 60dB, and reduces noise from 250 Hz to 100Hz by ~ 50dB). A HEPA filter was run constantly inside the behavioral box to both provide a constant source of white noise and to clean the air of particulates and odors. The open-field behavioral tasks were recorded via a ceiling mounted infrared camera. Ethovision’s behavioral analysis software was used to analyze the behavior including animal tracking, animal speed, and animal sniffs of the objects (Supplementary Fig. 3be).
To examine spatial memory consolidation, we tested 14 female mice (C57BL/6 mice, aged aged 12 weeks to 24 weeks) on the Object-Location Memory task (Supplementary Fig. 3ab). This task specifically tested hippocampal-dependent spatial memory without needing an aversive stimulus 70 (link),71 (link). This task leveraged the fact that mice naturally spend more time exploring novel objects and the fact that mice notice when objects have been moved to a new location. Mice were exposed to an open field arena with two distinct objects for 10 minutes. Half the mice (n = 7) were given 25 mg/kg ketamine and the other half (n = 7) were given a control injection. The following day, one of the objects was moved to a new spatial location and the mice were allowed to explore freely for 5 minutes. If the animal remembered the previous day, it would spend more time investigating the object that had moved. The animals were recorded using an infrared camera mounted above the arena, and the amount of time spent sniffing the objects was scored using Ethovision’s behavior tracking software. The videos were analyzed and the discrimination index (DI = {[time spent exploring object in novel location – time spent exploring object in familiar location]/total time exploring both objects} during the test phase was calculated as a measure of spatial memory.
In order to test the dose-response curve of ketamine, mice were given ketamine at the following doses: 0 mg/ kg, 10 mg/kg, 12.5 mg/kg, 15 mg/kg, 20 mg/kg, and 25 mg/kg once a day intraperitoneally (IP) (Supplementary Fig. 3d). The drug was delivered to mice through an IP injection and then immediately placed in the open field arena to be tracked for 30 minutes afterwards using Ethovision’s behavior tracking software. Different doses of ketamine (1 – 25 mg/kg) were used to generate a dose-response curve of the total distance the animal traveled during a recording session (Supplementary Fig. 3). Angular acceleration (radians/s2) was also calculated to quantify the stereotypic post-ketamine spinning behavior. The mouse underwent only a single injection with a single concentration of the drug on any given day. The number of lifetime injections was limited to 20 per animal and no more than 5 injections per week (one per day).
Publication Preprint 2023
Acceleration Animals Discrimination, Psychology Females Ketamine Males Memory Memory Consolidation Mice, House Mice, Inbred C57BL Odors Pharmaceutical Preparations Range of Motion, Articular Spatial Memory
In order to assess spatial reference memory, the Morris water maze test (MWM) [52 (link)] was used as previously described [41 (link)]. The apparatus consisted of a circular pool (ø 110 cm) and a small escape platform (ø 10 cm). The pool was filled with opaque water in order to make the platform invisible and to facilitate the video tracking and mice were trained to learn to localize the position of the submerged platform. Initially, a 3 day “cued training” session was carried out (4 trials per day), in which the platform position was marked with a visible triangular flag. Next, proximal cues were added around the pool and 5 days of “acquisition training” (4 trials per day) were performed 24 h after the last trial of the cue training. The hidden platform remained stationary for each mouse in this training block with the triangular flag removed. After a period of 24 h after the last trial of the acquisition training, a “probe trial” was carried to assess spatial reference memory. During this final 60 sec trial, proximal and distal cues remained attached to the pool, while the platform was removed. Mice with successful spatial reference memory consolidation were expected to show a preference for the target quadrant, as the platform location was kept constant during the entire acquisition training phase. To prevent hypothermia, mice were kept under infra-red light to dry in between the trials. Swimming paths were recorded with video-tracking software (ANY-maze, Stoelting Europe) allowing the analysis of further parameters such as latency to first entry into the platform/target quadrant, time into the platform/target quadrant, entries into the platform/target quadrant, swimming speed, quadrant preference or latency.
Full text: Click here
Publication 2023
Infrared Rays MAZE protocol Memory Consolidation Morris Water Maze Test Mus Spatial Memory
The PTSD model was induced using a Fear Conditioning apparatus [44 (link)]. The animals were placed in a soundproof cage for conditioning (Ugo Basile, Italy) with dimensions of 55 cm × 60 cm × 57 cm. Such a cage is equipped with a light emitting bulb, a loudspeaker and a floor made of metal rods to conduct electricity. The conditioned, neutral stimulus (CS) was the sound signal emitted by the loudspeaker inside the cage, with an intensity of 80 dB and a duration of 28 s. Additionally, the striped walls and the floor provided a context (context A) which, like the sound signal, constituted a CS. The sound signal and the context were associated with the aversive US (the irritation of the animals’ paws with an electric current of 1.5 mA) for the last 2 s after the sound signal. Then, after a short break, the procedure was repeated. Thus, during each training session, the animals were subjected to two sequences of the CS–US. The animal was then left in a soundproof cage for 60 s for memory consolidation, after that, the rat was returned to its home cage. Half of the studied animals (n = 14) were treated with electric shock (stressed), while the other half was treated only with CS (non-stressed). The experimental procedures were performed according to previously described methods [84 (link)] with modifications. The timeline of the research procedures is presented in Figure 1.
Full text: Click here
Publication 2023
6-pyruvoyl-tetrahydropterin synthase deficiency Animals CM 55 Electricity Fear Light Medulla Oblongata Memory Consolidation Metals Rod Photoreceptors Shock Sound TimeLine
There are several electrical brain waves, although researchers still do not fully agree on the exact frequency range and functions of the brain waves. In this study, four of these bands were quantified: the delta band (ranging between 2 to 4 Hz), theta band (ranging between 4 and 8 Hz), alpha band (ranging between 8 and 12 Hz), and beta band (ranging between 12 and 30 Hz) [20 ]. Of the four, (1) delta is the slowest frequency band with the highest power and associates with sleep states and memory consolidation; (2) the theta band is related to a state of drowsiness, network coordination, and episodic memory; (3) the alpha band is responsible for cognitive performance, selective attention, and consolidation of new motor sequences, and (4) beta is the faster frequency band with the lower power, which relates to sensorimotor association, motor imagery, and autonomous nervous system regulation [21 (link)].
The EEG technique is already seen as a robust biomarker for several medical conditions [22 (link)], although in the sports domain, due to the environment in which the measures are taken, it is still too early to define reliable biomarkers precisely [21 (link)]. Nevertheless, research in clinical [23 (link)] and sports [24 (link)] domains reported results in relative amplitude, a measure that seeks to present the contribution of oscillations in the different frequency ranges.
During the experiment, the participants performed the physical exercise task in a room where intermittent noise was allowed to maintain the ecology of the task [25 (link)]. A 32-channel Epoc Flex system (EPOC Flex Control Box from Emotiv, San Francisco, CA, USA) with various electrode placements within a traditional head cap was used. It was already validated against a Neuroscan device, but for event-related potentials (ERP), changes in alpha signatures, and steady-state visual-evoked potentials [26 (link)]. The EEG signals were recorded according to the international 10–20 system (Fp1, Fp2, F7, F3, Fz, F4, F8, T9, C5, C1, C2, C6, T10, T7, C3, Cz, C4, T8, P5, P1, P2, P6, P7, P3, Pz, P4, P8, O9, O1, Oz, O2, O10), with a sampling frequency of 128 Hz. The reference was the average of the left and right mastoids and the signals were recorded by EmotivPro (Emotiv Premium License, San Francisco, USA). During the task, EEG activity was recorded from all 32 channels. The circuit impedance was kept below 5 kΩ for all electrodes before the sessions. Participants were asked to sit comfortably in the leg press, the head remaining as still as possible, and to avoid excessive blinking.
Full text: Click here
Publication 2023
Attention Biological Markers Brain Waves Cognition Electricity Head Imagery, Guided Medical Devices Memory, Episodic Memory Consolidation Potentials, Event-Related Process, Mastoid Sleep Somnolence Systems, Nervous Vision Visual Evoked Potential
The main goal of this study was to further examine the effects of odor cueing on memory consolidation. To serve this objective, four different conditions were tested. The conditions differed in the application of rose odor during learning, testing and sleep (see table in Fig. 1). The N Condition (No odor) was the control condition in which no odor was applied in any phase. The median age of participants in this condition was 24 with a range between 18 and 33 years, 9 participants were males. In the LS (odor during Learning and Sleep; participants’ median age was 25 with a range between 19 and 34 years, 8 male) and LT Condition (odor during Learning and Testing; participants’ median age was 25 with a range between 19 and 35 years, 10 male), participants received odor during learning and either during sleep (LS) or during testing (LT). Participants of the LST Condition (Learning, Sleep and Testing; participants’ median age was 23 with a range between 18 and 31 years, 8 male) received rose odor during learning, testing, and sleep. Importantly, in Conditions LT and LST the odor cues were presented during Intermediate Tests–within the Learning Period–as well as during the final tests in the Final Test Period.

Experimental setup. Day 1 consisted of 2 Leaning Sessions, with one Learning Session consisting of a learning part which was followed by a testing part. Day 2, 3 and 4 started with the SFA questionnaire (Q). On day 2 and 3, the questionnaire was followed by the Learning Session, which started with the testing part and was followed by the learning part (order is switched compared to day 1). On day 4 the Final Test period started after the questionnaire with the first Final Test. Participants conducted Final Tests on day 4, day 10 and on day 31. The table with pictograms on the bottom left summarizes the four experimental conditions. The icons therein are adopted from Neumann et al. (2020).

The participants were randomly assigned to the different conditions. The participants were not informed about the condition in which they were tested.
Full text: Click here
Publication 2023
Males Memory Consolidation Odors Sleep

Top products related to «Memory Consolidation»

Sourced in Spain
The SMART-LD program is a software application designed to provide data analysis and visualization capabilities for laboratory equipment. The core function of this program is to facilitate the processing and interpretation of data generated by various lab instruments.
Sourced in United States, Denmark, United Kingdom
C57BL/6 mice are a widely used inbred mouse strain. They are a commonly used laboratory mouse model for a variety of research applications.
Sourced in United States
MATLAB is a high-level programming language and numerical computing environment used for a wide range of applications, including data analysis, algorithm development, and visualization. MATLAB version R2017b includes updates and enhancements to the core MATLAB environment, toolboxes, and integration with other software.
Sourced in United States, United Kingdom, Belgium, Germany, Canada, France, China, Australia, India, Italy, Switzerland, Spain, Denmark, Portugal, Japan, Austria, New Zealand, Panama, Netherlands, Norway, Thailand
DMSO (Dimethyl Sulfoxide) is a versatile solvent commonly used in various laboratory applications. It has a high boiling point and is miscible with water and many organic solvents. DMSO serves as an effective medium for dissolving a wide range of chemical compounds, making it a valuable tool in research and development settings.
Sourced in United States, Germany, United Kingdom, France, China, Italy, Canada
Norepinephrine is a laboratory product produced by Merck Group. It is a neurotransmitter and hormone that plays a role in the sympathetic nervous system. The core function of Norepinephrine is to regulate physiological processes such as heart rate, blood pressure, and pupil dilation.
Salvinorin B is a chemical compound that is a naturally occurring diterpene found in the Mexican plant Salvia divinorum. It is the primary active constituent of the plant, responsible for its psychoactive effects. Salvinorin B is a potent and selective kappa opioid receptor agonist.
Sourced in United States, United Kingdom, Germany, Japan, Belgium, Austria, Spain, China
SPSS 25.0 is a statistical software package developed by IBM. It provides advanced analytical capabilities for data management, analysis, and visualization. The core function of SPSS 25.0 is to enable users to conduct a wide range of statistical tests and procedures, including regression analysis, hypothesis testing, and multivariate techniques.
Sourced in United States
The Automated Syringe Pump is a precision instrument designed to accurately dispense and withdraw fluids. It utilizes a stepper motor to control the linear movement of a syringe plunger, enabling precise and reproducible fluid delivery. The device can be programmed to deliver a wide range of flow rates and volumes, making it a versatile tool for various laboratory applications.
Sourced in Spain
The SMART-CS program is a software tool developed by Harvard Apparatus. It provides a comprehensive solution for controlling and monitoring various laboratory equipment and experimental processes.
Sourced in United States
The SMART Panlab is a comprehensive video tracking system designed for behavioral research. It captures and analyzes animal movement and behavior in real-time, providing researchers with detailed data for their studies. The system utilizes advanced image processing algorithms to track the position, velocity, and other parameters of the subject within the defined experimental arena.

More about "Memory Consolidation"

Memory consolidation is a crucial cognitive process that transforms temporary memories into stable, long-term recollections.
This process involves the strengthening of neural connections and the reorganization of information within the brain.
Understanding the mechanisms behind memory consolidation is an active area of research, with implications for learning, cognitive function, and the development of treatments for memory-related disorders.
Factors such as sleep, stress, and neurochemical activity can influence the memory consolidation process.
For example, the neurotransmitter norepinephrine has been linked to the enhancement of memory consolidation, while the hallucinogenic compound Salvinorin B has been shown to impair this process in C57BL/6 mice.
Researchers often utilize a variety of tools and techniques to study memory consolidation, including the SMART-LD program, which assesses learning and memory in laboratory animals, and the SMART-CS program, which focuses on cognitive function.
Data analysis may be conducted using software like MATLAB (version R2017b) and SPSS (version 25.0), and experiments may involve the use of automated syringe pumps and the solvent DMSO to administer compounds.
By understanding the underlying mechanisms of memory consolidation, scientists can work towards developing more effective treatments for memory disorders, such as Alzheimer's disease and post-traumatic stress disorder.
The insights gained from this research may also have broader implications for enhancing cognitive abilities and optimizing learning processes.