The largest database of trusted experimental protocols

Eyelink 1000 desktop mount system

Manufactured by SR Research
Sourced in Canada

The EyeLink 1000 Desktop mount system is a high-performance eye-tracking solution designed for research and clinical applications. It is a self-contained unit that captures and records eye movements with high accuracy and precision. The system uses an infrared camera to track the user's eye and provides detailed data on gaze position, pupil size, and blink rate.

Automatically generated - may contain errors

9 protocols using eyelink 1000 desktop mount system

1

Visual Stimuli Presentation and Eye Tracking

Check if the same lab product or an alternative is used in the 5 most similar protocols
Stimuli were presented on a 23.6-inch VIEWPixx/EEG color monitor (1920 × 1080, 100 Hz) and generated with a custom made program written in MATLAB and the Psychophysics Toolbox (Pelli, 1997) running on a Dell Precision T1600 machine (Windows 7 Enterprise). Eye fixation was monitored with an Eyelink 1000 Desktop Mount system (SR Research, Ontario, Canada).
+ Open protocol
+ Expand
2

Multimodal Perception: Visual and Tactile Stimuli

Check if the same lab product or an alternative is used in the 5 most similar protocols
Visual stimuli were presented on a calibrated 32″ Display++ LCD monitor (Cambridge Research Systems, Rochester, UK) with a spatial resolution of 1920 × 1080 pixels and a refresh rate of 120 Hz (non-interlaced) using the Psychophysics Toolbox (Brainard, 1997 (link); Kleiner, 2010 ) in Matlab (The Mathworks, Inc., Natick, MA, USA). The background was average gray. Participants sat at a table in a darkened room with their head stabilized on a chin rest. The eye monitor distance was 100 cm, leading to a display size of 41° × 23°. Luminance of white and black pixels was 112.7 and 0.1 cd/m², respectively, as measured with a CS-2000 Spectroradiometer (Konica Minolta, Tokyo, Japan). Tactile stimuli were applied by custom-made vibrotactile devices (Engineering Acoustics Inc., Casselberry, FL, USA). They were attached on the tip of both index fingers using silicone finger sleeves. Participants comfortably rested their hands shoulder-width apart on foam pads in front of them. Because of the setup for tactile stimulation, manual response input was excluded. Thus we used gaze positions as response input. Eye positions were recorded using an SR Research Eyelink 1000 Desktop Mount system (SR Research Ltd., Mississauga, Ontario, Canada).
+ Open protocol
+ Expand
3

Binocular Eye Tracking with High-Speed CRT

Check if the same lab product or an alternative is used in the 5 most similar protocols
Stimuli were presented with a 140-Hz refresh rate on a 21-in. ViewSonic cathode-ray tube (CRT) monitor positioned 90 cm from participants, taking up a 24.8° × 18.6° (width × height) field of view. A chin and forehead rest was used to keep the participants’ head position stable. During stimulus presentation, the eye movements of the participants were recorded binocularly with an SR Research EyeLink 1000 Desktop mount system with high accuracy (0.15° best, 0.25–0.5° typical) and high precision (0.01° RMS). The Eyelink 1000 was equipped with the 2,000-Hz camera upgrade, allowing for binocular recordings at a sampling rate of 1,000 Hz per eye. The experiments were programmed in MATLAB (The MathWorks, Natick, MA, USA) using the OpenGL-based Psychophysics Toolbox 3 (PTB-3; Brainard, 1997 (link); Kleiner et al., 2007 ), which incorporates the EyeLink Toolbox extensions (F. W. Cornelissen et al., 2002 (link)). A game controller was used to record participants’ behavioral responses.
+ Open protocol
+ Expand
4

Minimizing Latency in Gaze-Contingent Displays

Check if the same lab product or an alternative is used in the 5 most similar protocols
Working with gaze-contingent displays requires minimizing the latency of the system (Loschky & Wolverton, 2007 (link); Saunders & Woods, 2014 (link)). Moreover, gaze-contingent manipulations of foveal vision call for eye-tracking equipment with high spatial accuracy and precision (Geringswald, Baumgartner, & Pollmann, 2013 (link)). Participants’ eye movements were recorded binocularly with an EyeLink 1000 Desktop mount system (SR Research, Ottawa, ON, Canada) with high accuracy (0.15° best, 0.25° to 0.5° typical) and high precision (0.01° root-mean-square [RMS]). The Eyelink 1000 was equipped with the 2000 Hz camera upgrade, allowing for binocular recordings at a sampling rate of 1000 Hz per eye. Stimuli were presented on a 21-inch CRT monitor with a refresh rate of 140 Hz at a viewing distance of 90 cm, taking up a 24.8° × 18.6° (width × height) field of view. A chin and forehead rest was used to keep the participants’ head position stable.
The experiments were programmed in MATLAB 2013a (The MathWorks, Natick, MA, USA) using the OpenGL-based Psychophysics Toolbox 3 (Brainard, 1997 (link); Kleiner, Brainard, & Pelli, 2007 ), which incorporates the EyeLink Toolbox extensions (F. W. Cornelissen, Peters, & Palmer, 2002 (link)). A game controller was used to record participants’ behavioral responses.
+ Open protocol
+ Expand
5

Eye Movements in Scene Viewing

Check if the same lab product or an alternative is used in the 5 most similar protocols
Eye movements were recorded using an EyeLink 1000 Desktop mount system (SR-Research, Ottawa, ON, Canada). It was equipped with the 2000 Hz camera upgrade, allowing for binocular recordings at a sampling rate of 1000 Hz for each eye. Data from the right eye were analysed. The experiment was implemented with the SR Research Experiment Builder software.
Each participant viewed 150 colour photographs of real-world scenes (Fig. 1a), which were presented in random order. The scene images were displayed on a 21-inch CRT monitor at a screen resolution of 800 × 600 pixels (width × height). Head position and viewing distance were fixed at 90 cm from the screen using a chin rest. Accordingly, scenes subtended 25.78° × 19.34°. Before the onset of each scene, a central fixation check was performed. Afterwards, the scene was displayed for 6 s during which participants were free to move their eyes. To provide a common task across participants, they were informed that, on a given trial, they would view a real-life scene and that this may be followed by a question asking them to recall a specific detail of the scene. On 30 trials, a test question asking about the presence or absence of a particular object appeared after scene presentation to probe participants’ scene encoding.
+ Open protocol
+ Expand
6

Eye Tracking with EyeLink 1000 System

Check if the same lab product or an alternative is used in the 5 most similar protocols
Using the EyeLink® 1000 Desktop Mount system (SR Research Ltd., Ontario, Canada), eye position and eye movements were determined by measuring the corneal reflection and dark pupil with a video-based infrared camera and an infrared reflective mirror. The eye tracker had a spatial resolution of 0.01° of the visual angle, and the signal was sampled and stored at a rate of 1000 Hz. Although the viewing was binocular, the recording was monocular (a standard procedure in eye-tracking studies). Calibration and validation of the measurements were performed before each experimental session.
+ Open protocol
+ Expand
7

Visual Object Search Experiment Protocol

Check if the same lab product or an alternative is used in the 5 most similar protocols
Twenty-two participants (mean age: 32.5 years; 11 women, 11 men) with normal or corrected-to-normal vision took part in the study. Eighteen participants reported to be right-handed (4 left-handed, all women). All participants gave their informed consent before the experiment. The Psychology Department at the University of Edinburgh granted ethics approval for the study, which conformed to the Declaration of Helsinki.
Stimuli were presented on a 21-inch CRT monitor at a viewing distance of 90 cm, with each scene image subtending a visual angle of 25.78° horizontally × 19.34° vertically. The experiment was implemented in SR Research Experiment Builder. Participants used a four-button Microsoft Sidewinder controller to indicate that they had found the target object.
Eye movements were recorded with an SR Research EyeLink 1000 Desktop mount system, which was equipped with the 2000 Hz camera upgrade, allowing for binocular recordings at a sampling rate of 1000 Hz for each eye. A chin rest with head support minimized head movement. We placed the chin rest such that the participant’s midpoint between the centers of the eyes was aligned with the vertical midline of the screen. We adjusted table height to place the participant’s straight-ahead view at the midpoint of the screen.
+ Open protocol
+ Expand
8

Eye movement tracking in psychophysics

Check if the same lab product or an alternative is used in the 5 most similar protocols
Eye movements were recorded with an EyeLink1000 desktop mount system (SR Research, Canada) at a sampling rate of 1000 Hz. Viewing was binocular, but only the position of the right eye was tracked. Saccades and fixations were extracted from raw gaze data during recording, by the EyeLink parser. Velocity and acceleration thresholds were set to the EyeLink default values of 30 degrees/s and 8000 degrees/s2 respectively. Stimulus presentation and response recording was controlled by MATLAB using Psychophysics Toolbox (Brainard, 1997 (link); Pelli, 1997 (link)).
+ Open protocol
+ Expand
9

Eye Tracking Protocol for Fixation Analysis

Check if the same lab product or an alternative is used in the 5 most similar protocols
Eye tracking was done using an Eyelink 1000 Desktop Mount system (SR Research, Ontario Canada) recording the right eye at 1000 Hz. Participants placed their head on a headrest supported under the chin and forehead to keep a stable position. Participants underwent a 13 point calibration/validation procedure at the beginning of each run (1 Baseline run, 1 Conditioning run and 3 runs of Generalization). The average mean-calibration error across all runs was Mean = 0.36°, Median = 0.34°, SD = 0.11. 91% of all runs had a calibration better than or equal to. 5°.
Fixation events were identified using commonly used parameter definitions (Wilming et al., 2017 (link)) (Eyelink cognitive configuration: saccade velocity threshold = 30°/second, saccade acceleration threshold = 8000° per second2, motion threshold = 0.1°). Fixation density maps (FDMs) were computed by spatially smoothing (Gaussian kernel of 1° of full width at half maximum) a 2D histogram of fixation locations, and were transformed to probability densities by normalizing to unit sum. FDMs included the center 500 × 500 pixels, including all facial elements where fixations were mostly concentrated (~95% of all fixations).
+ Open protocol
+ Expand

About PubCompare

Our mission is to provide scientists with the largest repository of trustworthy protocols and intelligent analytical tools, thereby offering them extensive information to design robust protocols aimed at minimizing the risk of failures.

We believe that the most crucial aspect is to grant scientists access to a wide range of reliable sources and new useful tools that surpass human capabilities.

However, we trust in allowing scientists to determine how to construct their own protocols based on this information, as they are the experts in their field.

Ready to get started?

Sign up for free.
Registration takes 20 seconds.
Available from any computer
No download required

Sign up now

Revolutionizing how scientists
search and build protocols!