Job resources were assessed with four scales: decision authority, social support, work-time control, and reward. Decision authority and social support were derived from the reliable and well-tested Demand–Control–Support-Questionnaire (DCSQ) (Chungkham et al., 2013 (link); Theorell et al., 1988 (link)), with response alternative on a 4-degree Likert scale ranging from 1: No, (almost) never to 4: Yes, often. Decision authority was measured with a 2-item-scale (what to do at work and how to do the work). The Scale reliability coefficient was 0.76. Social support was assessed with a 5-item-scale regarding pleasant atmosphere, understanding and cohesiveness among co-workers and managers. Cronbach's alpha was 0.85. Work-time control was measured with a 6-item-scale (Ala-Mursula et al., 2002 (link)), assessing the opportunities to influence the working time (length of work day, start and end times, taking breaks, running private errands during worktime, which days to work, and vacations) with five response alternatives ranging from 1: no, to a very small extent to 5: yes, to a large extent. Cronbach's alpha was 0.82. Rewards were measured by a 7-item-scale (Li et al., 2019 (link)) concerning job promotions (adequate salary, work and promotion prospects), esteem (receiving the earned acknowledgement, prestige and respect), and job security (including not expecting or experiences undesirable job changes) with response alternative on a 4-degree Likert scale ranging from 1: no, not at all to 4: yes, completely agree. Cronbach's alpha was 0.64. A sum-index was estimated for each job resource. Thereafter, they were dichotomized into binary indicators according to their respective median values: decision authority: 0 “low” (37.5%; score range 2–5) or 1 “high” (62.5%; score range 6–8), social support: 0 “low” (51.5%; score range 5–15) or 1 “high” (48.5%; score range 16–20), work-time control: 0 “low” (52.5%; score range 6–13) or 1 “high” (47.5%; score range 14–30), and rewards: 0 “low” (44.7%; score range 7–17) or 1 “high” (55.3%; score range 18–28) (Table 1). Job resources were used as moderator variables in the association between physical demands or physical hazards and working longer.
Descriptive statistics and bivariate correlations with regard to the observations.
Table 1
1
2
3
4
5
Nobs
Nlowest (%)
Cut-off value (range)
1 Physical demands
–
2729
53.39
10 (3–18)
2 Physical hazards
.279***
–
2696
53.75
2 (0–6)
3 Decision authority
−.035†
−.073***
–
2744
37.46
6 (2–8)
4 Social support
−.055**
−.147***
.144***
–
2669
51.48
16 (5–20)
5 WTC
−.028
.023
.244***
.100***
–
2593
52.45
14 (6–30)
6 Rewards
−.067**
−.101***
.181***
.306***
.157***
2262
44.69
18 (7–28)
Note. All variables are dichotomized. Nobs for number of observations (transitions). Nlowest for the percent of observations in the category below the cut-off value. *** for p < .001; ** for p < .01; * for p < .05; † for 0.05 ≤ p < .10.
Age category (59–63 years (0) versus ≥64 years (1) at baseline). This cut-off was based on the fact that the older age category included observations where the individual approached 65 years (representing the Swedish normative retirement age) and consequently two years later (at follow-up) had passed the normative retirement age.
Stengård J., Leineweber C, & Wang H.X. (2023). Prolonging working life among blue-collar workers: The buffering effect of psychosocial job resources on the association between physically demanding and hazardous work and retirement timing. SSM - Population Health, 22, 101372.
The job resources (decision authority, social support, work-time control, and rewards) were used as moderator variables in the association between physical demands or physical hazards and working longer, but were not explicitly mentioned as independent variables.
Annotations
Based on most similar protocols
Etiam vel ipsum. Morbi facilisis vestibulum nisl. Praesent cursus laoreet felis. Integer adipiscing pretium orci. Nulla facilisi. Quisque posuere bibendum purus. Nulla quam mauris, cursus eget, convallis ac, molestie non, enim. Aliquam congue. Quisque sagittis nonummy sapien. Proin molestie sem vitae urna. Maecenas lorem.
As authors may omit details in methods from publication, our AI will look for missing critical information across the 5 most similar protocols.
About PubCompare
Our mission is to provide scientists with the largest repository of trustworthy protocols and intelligent analytical tools, thereby offering them extensive information to design robust protocols aimed at minimizing the risk of failures.
We believe that the most crucial aspect is to grant scientists access to a wide range of reliable sources and new useful tools that surpass human capabilities.
However, we trust in allowing scientists to determine how to construct their own protocols based on this information, as they are the experts in their field.
Ready to
get started?
Sign up for free.
Registration takes 20 seconds.
Available from any computer
No download required